Call 855-808-4530 or email [email protected] to receive your discount on a new subscription.
A certain amount of controversy has arisen over the past decade concerning the litigant's right to preserve on audio media the interviews of mental-health professionals engaged in expert custody and parenting time assessments. While the majority of our jurisdictions now have case law to support the premise that audio-taping is mandatory upon request, there has always been a measure of resistance to the concept, generally emanating from a relatively small percentage of practicing custody experts.
When questioned about their reluctance or refusal to conduct audio-taped custody and parenting time assessments, the experts' responses have been fairly typical, albeit falling far short of containing any sort of scientific validity. Objections based on the argument that the taping would have some sort of “chilling effect,” or that the interviewees would likely “perform for the tape” are frequently raised, often with the resistant expert adding that many of his or her “colleagues feel the same way.” This sort of facile, non-scientific “argument” should be summarily rejected in all of our jurisdictions.
Opponents to such objections have often speculated that the real motivation behind certain experts' resistance to audio-taping is their underlying desire to eliminate additional sources of impeachment evidence at trial. Without taping, we are forced to rely primarily upon the expert's notes and memories, both of which are typically flawed to various extents. Most psychologists, for that matter, do not even start writing until after the session is over. Attorneys are expected to trust in their methodology, how they carry it out in practice, and the accuracy of their data-gathering and reporting ' all this without the benefit of an objective quality-assurance mechanism. This is far too much to reasonably expect.
Objective Evidence
We need objective evidence, not only to assure that the examining expert is abiding by any professional guidelines that may exist and apply, but also that the expert had not committed any memory errors, and he or she has not corrupted the memories of any subject children in the course of the examination process.
Generally speaking, memory is a record of: 1) pattern-recognition; 2) interpretive analysis; and 3) source information. We will only deal with the third component, for purposes of this example.
Source Information
Failure to identify the actual root of information acquisition properly is known as a source misattribution error. While adults are certainly prone to this type of memory error (20%-35%), preschoolers are disproportionately vulnerable (50%-70%). Leading causes of source misattributions in children are:
Interviewer Bias
Audio-taping is an indispensable tool for memorializing evidence of possible interviewer bias. Interviewer bias is one of the driving forces behind suggestive interviewing and flawed investigations. Now a widely recognized and accepted concept among experts in the field of children's suggestibility, interviewer bias characterizes those interviewers who hold an a priori belief about the occurrence or non-occurrence of certain events and, as a result, mold the interview to elicit statements from the interviewee that are consistent with the interviewer's beliefs, rather than elicit the facts supporting what may have actually happened.
The presence of interviewer bias is often revealed through the interviewer's use of several improper interviewing techniques, which recent research has overwhelmingly shown to be suggestive and dangerous to the truth-finding process. These techniques include:
Research has shown that an interviewer's beliefs about an event influences the accuracy of children's answers, particularly if the interviewer is an adult of perceived high status. Children are sensitive to the status of their interviewer. A child's recognition of a power differential between himself and an interviewer of high status will increase his vulnerability to suggestions. Children view these adults as truthful and will conform their answers to what the interviewer is looking for and defer to the interviewer if he or she challenges their reports.
Source Monitoring Problems
Source monitoring problems can also result from flawed interview technique, particularly when there are multiple interview sessions protracted over time. The source monitoring problem refers to the inability of children to maintain a mental distinction between what the child has actually experienced as opposed to what the child has learned or imagined from some other source. Sometimes, when children are asked to pretend about a certain event, they later have difficulty determining where the idea came from, and whether the event is true or false.
Recent Research
When one adds parental influence and pressure to the above mentioned discredited techniques, as well as any other significant issues going on in a child's life, the combination is a perfect storm for unreliable disclosures. Failure to preserve an audio account of the interviews effectively hobbles any attempt to check for the use of these bias-reflecting techniques.
Recent research has likewise proven that the following interview techniques are unnecessarily suggestive and improper. The use of them in child custody and parenting time evaluations creates a substantial risk that a child's disclosures will be unreliable. Although there are others, such as interviews with adults of high status, use of anatomically detailed dolls and drawings, source monitoring problems and peer pressure, the potentially toxic techniques listed below are limited to those that can be disclosed through audio-taping:
A single suggestive interviewing technique has a deleterious effect on the reliability of a child's disclosure. When several of these techniques are used together, the likelihood that a child's disclosure will be unreliable increases significantly. Recent research has shown conclusively that a combination of suggestive techniques used during an interview is likely to produce high assent rates for false events. Suggestive interviewing affects the credibility of children's statements to such an extent that even a professional may not be able to determine when suggestively interviewed children are providing a false report. It is therefore critically important that all forensic interviews be captured on audio media, or video media, where permissible.
Part Two of this article will discuss the ways to address the interviewer's objections to audio-taping.
Curtis J. Romanowski, a member of this newsletter's Board of Editors, limits his practice to NJ Divorce & NJ Matrimonial Law, NJ Child Custody Law & NJ Family Law. An experienced and commanding Trial Attorney since 1980, as Chairman of New Jersey's Collaborative Family Institute, he is dedicated to the positive transformation of New Jersey divorce, child custody & parenting disputes.
A certain amount of controversy has arisen over the past decade concerning the litigant's right to preserve on audio media the interviews of mental-health professionals engaged in expert custody and parenting time assessments. While the majority of our jurisdictions now have case law to support the premise that audio-taping is mandatory upon request, there has always been a measure of resistance to the concept, generally emanating from a relatively small percentage of practicing custody experts.
When questioned about their reluctance or refusal to conduct audio-taped custody and parenting time assessments, the experts' responses have been fairly typical, albeit falling far short of containing any sort of scientific validity. Objections based on the argument that the taping would have some sort of “chilling effect,” or that the interviewees would likely “perform for the tape” are frequently raised, often with the resistant expert adding that many of his or her “colleagues feel the same way.” This sort of facile, non-scientific “argument” should be summarily rejected in all of our jurisdictions.
Opponents to such objections have often speculated that the real motivation behind certain experts' resistance to audio-taping is their underlying desire to eliminate additional sources of impeachment evidence at trial. Without taping, we are forced to rely primarily upon the expert's notes and memories, both of which are typically flawed to various extents. Most psychologists, for that matter, do not even start writing until after the session is over. Attorneys are expected to trust in their methodology, how they carry it out in practice, and the accuracy of their data-gathering and reporting ' all this without the benefit of an objective quality-assurance mechanism. This is far too much to reasonably expect.
Objective Evidence
We need objective evidence, not only to assure that the examining expert is abiding by any professional guidelines that may exist and apply, but also that the expert had not committed any memory errors, and he or she has not corrupted the memories of any subject children in the course of the examination process.
Generally speaking, memory is a record of: 1) pattern-recognition; 2) interpretive analysis; and 3) source information. We will only deal with the third component, for purposes of this example.
Source Information
Failure to identify the actual root of information acquisition properly is known as a source misattribution error. While adults are certainly prone to this type of memory error (20%-35%), preschoolers are disproportionately vulnerable (50%-70%). Leading causes of source misattributions in children are:
Interviewer Bias
Audio-taping is an indispensable tool for memorializing evidence of possible interviewer bias. Interviewer bias is one of the driving forces behind suggestive interviewing and flawed investigations. Now a widely recognized and accepted concept among experts in the field of children's suggestibility, interviewer bias characterizes those interviewers who hold an a priori belief about the occurrence or non-occurrence of certain events and, as a result, mold the interview to elicit statements from the interviewee that are consistent with the interviewer's beliefs, rather than elicit the facts supporting what may have actually happened.
The presence of interviewer bias is often revealed through the interviewer's use of several improper interviewing techniques, which recent research has overwhelmingly shown to be suggestive and dangerous to the truth-finding process. These techniques include:
Research has shown that an interviewer's beliefs about an event influences the accuracy of children's answers, particularly if the interviewer is an adult of perceived high status. Children are sensitive to the status of their interviewer. A child's recognition of a power differential between himself and an interviewer of high status will increase his vulnerability to suggestions. Children view these adults as truthful and will conform their answers to what the interviewer is looking for and defer to the interviewer if he or she challenges their reports.
Source Monitoring Problems
Source monitoring problems can also result from flawed interview technique, particularly when there are multiple interview sessions protracted over time. The source monitoring problem refers to the inability of children to maintain a mental distinction between what the child has actually experienced as opposed to what the child has learned or imagined from some other source. Sometimes, when children are asked to pretend about a certain event, they later have difficulty determining where the idea came from, and whether the event is true or false.
Recent Research
When one adds parental influence and pressure to the above mentioned discredited techniques, as well as any other significant issues going on in a child's life, the combination is a perfect storm for unreliable disclosures. Failure to preserve an audio account of the interviews effectively hobbles any attempt to check for the use of these bias-reflecting techniques.
Recent research has likewise proven that the following interview techniques are unnecessarily suggestive and improper. The use of them in child custody and parenting time evaluations creates a substantial risk that a child's disclosures will be unreliable. Although there are others, such as interviews with adults of high status, use of anatomically detailed dolls and drawings, source monitoring problems and peer pressure, the potentially toxic techniques listed below are limited to those that can be disclosed through audio-taping:
A single suggestive interviewing technique has a deleterious effect on the reliability of a child's disclosure. When several of these techniques are used together, the likelihood that a child's disclosure will be unreliable increases significantly. Recent research has shown conclusively that a combination of suggestive techniques used during an interview is likely to produce high assent rates for false events. Suggestive interviewing affects the credibility of children's statements to such an extent that even a professional may not be able to determine when suggestively interviewed children are providing a false report. It is therefore critically important that all forensic interviews be captured on audio media, or video media, where permissible.
Part Two of this article will discuss the ways to address the interviewer's objections to audio-taping.
Curtis J. Romanowski, a member of this newsletter's Board of Editors, limits his practice to NJ Divorce & NJ Matrimonial Law, NJ Child Custody Law & NJ Family Law. An experienced and commanding Trial Attorney since 1980, as Chairman of New Jersey's Collaborative Family Institute, he is dedicated to the positive transformation of New Jersey divorce, child custody & parenting disputes.
ENJOY UNLIMITED ACCESS TO THE SINGLE SOURCE OF OBJECTIVE LEGAL ANALYSIS, PRACTICAL INSIGHTS, AND NEWS IN ENTERTAINMENT LAW.
Already a have an account? Sign In Now Log In Now
For enterprise-wide or corporate acess, please contact Customer Service at [email protected] or 877-256-2473
This article highlights how copyright law in the United Kingdom differs from U.S. copyright law, and points out differences that may be crucial to entertainment and media businesses familiar with U.S law that are interested in operating in the United Kingdom or under UK law. The article also briefly addresses contrasts in UK and U.S. trademark law.
The Article 8 opt-in election adds an additional layer of complexity to the already labyrinthine rules governing perfection of security interests under the UCC. A lender that is unaware of the nuances created by the opt in (may find its security interest vulnerable to being primed by another party that has taken steps to perfect in a superior manner under the circumstances.
With each successive large-scale cyber attack, it is slowly becoming clear that ransomware attacks are targeting the critical infrastructure of the most powerful country on the planet. Understanding the strategy, and tactics of our opponents, as well as the strategy and the tactics we implement as a response are vital to victory.
Possession of real property is a matter of physical fact. Having the right or legal entitlement to possession is not "possession," possession is "the fact of having or holding property in one's power." That power means having physical dominion and control over the property.
UCC Sections 9406(d) and 9408(a) are one of the most powerful, yet least understood, sections of the Uniform Commercial Code. On their face, they appear to override anti-assignment provisions in agreements that would limit the grant of a security interest. But do these sections really work?